This is a preprint of a review which appeared in February 2023 issue of Journal of Linguistics. Published by Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000524, link to the Version of Record (VOR) is the same as DOI.
Gita Martohardjono and Suzanne Flynn (eds.) Language in Development: A Crosslinguistic Perspective . Cambridge: The MIT Press, 2021. Pp. xi + 346.
Language in Development: A Crosslinguistic Perspective (LD) is a volume honoring Barbara Lust whose work spans across the subfields of first and second language acquisition, research methodology, work with hearing impaired, aphasia, &c. Scholarly work like Lust’s, which is truly cross-linguistic sensu incorporation of diverse intra-linguistic but cross-subfield perspectives, is exigent in modern linguistics, where the subfields grow increasingly oblivious to existence of other subfields. As such, second language acquisitionists will benefit from attending to what is going on with merge and phases; conversely, syntacticians will find that acquisition might be helpful in their debates on multidominance and antilocality. In such an environment, where a nascent science like linguistics faces crossroads, it is vital to sustain the value of convergence and holistic approach. LD celebrates just that. In what follows I overview the book and comment on select papers in the edited volume. Out of thirteen chapters, omitted are those by Virginia Valian, by Christina Dye & Claire Foley, and by Maria Blume.
Part I Theoretical and Methodological Considerations
D. Terence Langendoen’s chapter is a bracing excursion into disjunctive coordinate constructions. The inadequacy of the calculus of individuals (CI, Leonard and Goodman 1940) for disjunctive connectors is shown, and an extended CI ordering (ECI, I*) is proposed and then recursively defined. Langendoen’s theorizing is interesting and sound, yet the empirical value of it is not immediately clear. Eschewing much of the technical discussion for the sake of accessibility, let me only say that the topic of disjunction and basic Booleans generally is, while fundamental, not well-studied for natural language. One example is languages that disobey De Morgan’s law(s). Langendoen’s conclusion is that there is much complexity to disjunctive constructions in terms of logic and that children seemingly choose from hundreds of potential options when interpreting disjunction. I add that there is likely even more complexity should we attempt empirical investigations.
Hard Words, by the late Lila R. Gleitman and colleagues (LRG) delves into child language acquisition of vocabulary. The chapter is a summary of well-known work so I will only raise one question which challenges the ‘hard’ vs. ‘easy’ dichotomy. Before moving to ‘hard’ words, LRG deal with ‘easy’ ones stipulating that for nouns like cat, ‘all the learner has to do is to match the real-world environment (recurrent cat situations) with the sounds of the word (recurrent phonetic sequences) in the exposure language’ (33). To my view, this seeming straightforwardness is questionable. The learners are able to identify something as a cat even if they have never been faced with, say, a white cat while all they have seen in life are brown and black cats. Surely, a salient property of cat-ness as acquired by matching is missing from the white cat, yet the cat is identified as a cat, not as an unknown object. These kinds of generalizations are tricky to account for, and the seeming ‘easiness’ of acquisition of concrete nouns is, to my view, misleading. Hence, perhaps it is concrete nouns that are ‘hard’ and abstract nouns/verbs, &c. that are ‘easy’ – since for the latter we do have LRG’s theory of acquisition, and for the former we don’t?
Part II Children
Austin et al. report on a longitudinal study on five child heritage bilingual (Spanish/English) speakers and attrition of gender and number agreement. While the conclusions drawn by the author concern the possible onset of morphological attrition and question the role of language dominance, I agree with the remarks of Martohardjono (122f) who thinks some amount of nontarget production (which the conclusions are based on) can be accounted for by exposure of the five participants to different varieties of Spanish. Indeed, I am willing to go further and say that it is extremely hard to interpret the results of a study where the participants come from strikingly heterogeneous language backgrounds when it is coupled with such a small number of participants. Indeed, the authors do not overview differences between Spanishes of Peru, Honduras, and Ecuador where parents of the participants of the study come from. Yet, such differences are sure to influence children’s acquisition, hence attrition processes. Homogeneity – the degree of which can be debated – of participants’ background in any study is a requisite methodological element aiding meaningful interpretation of the study.
The chapter by Kedar is an interesting account of gestures (not signing) in child language within a longitudinal (3;0 - 3;8) case study on a Hebrew-English bilingual. While we need to be cautious interpreting the results of case studies, I heartily support the view that studies on externalization to diverse modalities bring an interesting perspective to the field of language acquisition, and – perhaps – are even able to contribute to syntactic studies investigating syntax-PF mapping. Kedar’s study, however, faces some challenges. The broad framework is that bilinguals will gesture more while acquiring the second native language per the temporary deficits in lexicon and grammar of the target language. Yet, I contend that before empirical investigation of such conjecture is attempted, it is imperative to develop the conjecture into a model and show the precise mechanisms of the gestures’ alleged compensatory function – hopefully, in a formally sufficient and developmentally adequate way. Until then, I am not entirely sure whether Kedar’s subject gestured more because she was compensating, because she was excited to speak to Kedar, or for some other reason.
Reiko Mazuka’s valuable contribution on infant-directed speech (IDS) draws some very fine distinctions which previous research in the field has not addressed. IDS is said to be conventionally slower than adult-directed speech, and this slowness (hence, overarticulation) has been a hallmark of some of the research in first language acquisition. Mazuko argues that the perceived ‘slowness’ could be a result of ‘averaging the syllable (or mora) duration without removing the contribution of phrase-final lengthening’ (195). Such averaging ‘has an effect of overestimating the syllable (or mora) duration’ (195). As Mazuka rightfully points out, this calls for re-evaluation of some of the research in the field. The findings could easily be extended to second language speech where similar methodological pitfalls apply. And even though I do not think that most domain-general models of L1/L2 acquisition are going to come under further scrutiny, Mazuka’s distinction is likely to be valuable for domain-specific models of acquisition (of phonology).
The chapter by Santelmann looks at discontinuous dependent morphemes (DDMs) in English and German. Not surprisingly (as she herself remarks on 221), Santelmann finds differences in parental input in these two languages: the distance between constituents separating connected morphemes and the kind of constituents are different in English and in German. I only aim to question Santelmann’s conclusions: she writes that ‘children cannot use a universal strategy for tracking morphosyntactic relationships within sentences’ (222). The data in the chapter points to German children identifying constituents, but Santelmann claims that English children need not do that since they can just ‘focus on individual lexical items’. However, the even if English children do not appear to use their cognizance of constituency, it does not mean they cannot use it. Moreover, even the conclusion that English children ‘need not focus’ on constituency is not entirely warranted: why not? Santelmann ties it to unnecessary ‘processing resources’, but I think additional studies are due to tease apart what really is processively burdensome, why, and in what manner. Only after those findings are available can the language-specific preferences for DMMs can be scrutinized.
Part III Adults
The chapter by Yao and Packard (YP) asks whether L2 learners parse ‘Verb NP1 de NP2’ constructions in Chinese as relative clauses (RC; proi NP1 de NP2 i) or in a verb-object manner, along the lines of [VP V [NP [PossP N Poss ] N ]]. YP overview previous studies and conduct their own experiments to show that native Chinese speakers prefer the RC-interpretation, while nonnative speakers from both head-initial and head-final languages do not analyze de-constructions as RCs, but as VOs. YP provide thorough statistical analysis and awareness of pitfalls of ANOVA while using it with Likert scale and the likes, thus providing alternative analyses. I concur with the conclusion drawn that there is no full or partial transfer. However, YP might benefit from looking also at the pro-drop parameter, since RC above requires a pro to establish a dependency. Transfer actually might be detected there. Further, I vehemently disagree with the overall conclusion of the study that parsing strategies may be ‘qualitatively different’ (250) for L1 and L2. Yes, L2 learners do not parse de-RCs in the same way as native speakers do, but that only indicates that they have not achieved ultimate attainment; not that there are fundamental differences in L1 and L2 processing. A qualitative/fundamental difference would be presented by a different parsing mechanism, e.g. left-corner parser with top-down prediction component vs. ‘active’ bottom-up parsers (see work by Masaya Yoshida). The authors do give a nod to the fact that ‘proficiency [could have] contributed to the nonnative parsing decisions’ (150). As proficiency generally tends to account for all things nonnative, this should be the null hypothesis and just a nod does not resolve the potential objection. Further, the information about the background of the L2 participants and testing which was performed is very sparse. A score of 65 out of 100 on some potentially lacking in validity or/and reliability Chinese language test potentially lacking in validity or/and reliability and describing the learners as ‘advanced’ says exactly nothing about the proficiency of the learners. ‘Advanced’, ‘intermediate’ are just labels which when not supplied by ample descriptors are empty. Yet, obliviousness to requisite validity of language assessment is a more than endemic problem in SLA research, far from being peculiar to this study.
Martohardjono, Valian, and Klein (MVK) take up the deficit and transfer accounts (d/t) of L2 acquisition in their chapter while looking at acquisition of tense. Personally, I would only say that d/t’s claims (about Universal Grammar for example) presuppose that what is acquired is a non-human language so they are investigating something entirely different from the faculty of language in the Chomskyan sense. MVK, however, are willing to go much further and engage with the d/t in a meaningful manner. They provide a careful overview of studies up to date and show that d/t are not compatible with observed evidence for young learners, not only for advanced or ‘near-native’ learners (which has been a hallmark of d/t accounts). I applaud MVK’s crusade while treading very gingerly regarding their reliance on frequency and saliency – those constructs are not yet entirely (well-)defined (saliency in particular) or linked to formally sufficient accounts (frequency), so we need to exercise extreme caution while making use of them.
Phillips et al. (IP) look at heritage Spanish speakers and late ESP-ENG bilinguals’ differences in processing. They provide a comprehensive account of existing literature and get at the right questions, some of which I highlight below. First, they draw attention to the fact that there is no significant difference in susceptibility to L2 influence between heritage speakers and late bilinguals (310). More importantly, heritage speakers who acquired L1 and L2 in similar environments and perform similarly on proficiency tests do not necessarily constitute a homogeneous sample. I stressed the vitally requisite homogeneity elsewhere in the review, and IP are consonant with that. Lastly, as with most groups under psycholinguistic spotlight, ‘heritage speakers’ are defined very loosely and research on different populations are being grouped under the same label, pointing to only superficially contradicting results. More care with terminology and who the terminology is exercised upon is exigent, and IP offer evidence to support that.
Lastly, Sherman and Flynn overview the available linguistic research on early language changes in Alzheimer’s disease and offer an important methodological point to bear in mind for clinical research: the former can inform the latter in valuable ways. The methodologies used in first language acquisition studies are entirely applicable for studies of subjects with mild cognitive impairment, to which their experiments investigating free relatives, ellipsis in coordinate sentences, &c. richly attest. The cross-discipline applications that open the horizons of new insight are inspiring and S&F’s ingenuity in driving them is admirable.
Conclusive remarks
Despite the critiques above, the volume achieves what it sets out to achieve: under a common theme of language development, studies from different areas of linguistics are combined and made mutually relevant. Semantics, syntax, processing, L1/L2/heritage/bilingual acquisition, methodology, clinical linguistics all come together in an overall successful attempt at cross-linguistic volume in the spirit of Barbara Lust! I stand staunchly with Martohardjono and Flynn in advocating for cross-field cooperation, and I see enormous benefits in future work, the objective of which is to point out different, bracing, unconventional, not immediately observable perspectives on – for some old, and for some new – phenomena. In fact, I think this is the only way forward.
Author’s address:
Benjamin W. Slivka Hall,
Northwestern University,
Evanston, Illinois, USA 60201
doz@u.northwestern.edu